What is the ventral attention network?

The VAN is a unilateral, typically right hemispheric brain network responsible for swapping our focus between various tasks and responding to unexpected cues, making it pivotal to daily function1

As the brain’s radar, the ventral attention network, (sometimes referred to as the visuospatial attention network) is frequently activated in relation to the other major attentional network, the Dorsal Attention Network (DAN). Where the DAN could be considered “the lens”, responsible for focussing and guiding attention toward a salient stimulus, the VAN plays a more pronounced role in pivoting our attention when new information is introduced. 

Imagine walking through a garden on a hot summer’s day. While walking through this garden your attention may focus solely on smelling the roses, however far more likely is that your attention would be grabbed along the way by new, more salient information: the humming of bees, the heat on the back of your neck, and other cues which arise on your journey that need to be interpreted to fully understand your situation. The VAN is the brain network enabling this interpretation, and by communicating with other brain networks such as the limbic system, visual, and auditory networks, enables our taking in of our complete surroundings.  

The human brain receives sensory input constantly, and it is the VAN’s role to juggle our attention between this information and ensure that we do not neglect to respond to important spatial, sensory, or cognitive information. Indeed, the inability to facilitate this is broadly classified as “Spatial Neglect”.  

To return to the analogy, as you continue to walk through the garden, your friend appears and passes you a ball. Though your limbic and auditory networks are still sending cues about the smell of the flowers and sounds of the bees, your attention must now shift and respond to the unexpected cue of the incoming ball. The VAN acts to respond to this new stimulus, recruiting the sensorimotor network and switching your attention towards catching the ball. After this conflict is resolved, the VAN switches your attention back to your previous state of reverie, enabling a smooth transition of high and low attention-demanding states. 

Discovery of the VAN

In 1984, Posner et al.,2 studied the effects of damage to the parietal lobe on attention. The results of the damage produced disengagement of attention – Spatial Neglect.2 However, this disengagement was not replicated by damage to the frontal, midbrain, or temporal lobes.2

At the turn of the 21st century, a seminal study by Corbetta and Shulman identified two separate, anatomically distinct attentional systems of the brain: the dorsal (DAN) and ventral (VAN) attention networks3. Whilst the DAN, they argued, guided allocation of attention to specific information, the VAN played a more prominent role in shifting attention to unexpected or new information1,4

From this core work, studies began using imaging techniques to evaluate spatial5 and visual attention6. Shifts in attention activated the superior parietal lobe, and further study evaluated how these areas correlate with the occipital lobe for efficient and inefficient visuospatial shifts of attention6-7.

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans, these theories of attention processing began to more closely unravel how the brain’s two attentional networks interact. Importantly, much like the Central Executive and Default Mode networks work in counterbalance to enable active (task-driven) and dormant (at rest) states respectively, so too do the VAN and DAN work in opposition during goal-directed and unexpected attentional states. Expected visual tasks presented in a series during functional magnetic resonance series activate the DAN, however, when an unexpected visual cue is added spontaneously into this series, the VAN is starkly activated8.

What is attention, exactly?

 “Attention” is arguably the highest-level cognitive process. In other words, it is possibly the most essential process underlying all sophisticated thinking skills. Its main components are alertness, selectivity, and processing capacity, and these varieties of attention permeate most cognitive processes1,2.

There are two modes of attentional processing: bottom-up and top-down attention. Whilst top-down attention, the selective choosing of where to allocate attention to achieve expectations and goals is controlled by the DAN, bottom-up attention is encoded by the VAN.

Bottom-up attention is an efficient shift of focussed attention in response to unanticipated  stimuli, such as motion, re-orientation, unexpected noises, or visual changes7,9-10. In the example of a friend passing a ball to you as you stroll through the garden, despite you still remaining focussed on walking, you may still turn your head to locate the ball’s position — even if you had been engrossed in your surroundings the moment before. The brain’s ventral attention network (VAN) is responsible for this bottom-up shift (more on this below).

To enable harmonious attention shifts, the VAN does not operate alone. Other brain networks also assist with stimulus response and unexpected attention-demanding responses.

Location of the VAN in the brain

While the VAN is unilaterally located in the right hemisphere, the DAN is a bilateral network. It demonstrates strong connectivity (see Connectomics) between areas in the lateral occipital lobe, the pre-central sulcus, the dorsal-most portion of the superior frontal sulcus considered to be the frontal eye fields (FEF), the ventral premotor cortex, superior parietal lobule, intraparietal sulcus, and motion-sensitive middle temporal area.5

 

VAN Axial View AXIAL VIEW

VAN Sagittal viewSAGITTAL VIEW

VAN Coronal View-1 CORONAL VIEW

Pathology resulting in changes to the VAN

Since the brain’s attentional networks are implicated in most major cognitive processes, degeneration and pathology in the brain can disrupt or cause damage to attentional capabilities.

Neurodegenerative diseases and strokes can result in impairment of top-down processing by the VAN, manifesting as spatial neglect16. Neglect can occur contralaterally, with patients suffering damage to the right hemisphere having difficulty noticing stimuli on the left side of space3,17. Neglect is most frequently associated with stroke, and can be more severe following right-hemisphere damage18. This syndrome manifests as the inability to orient or perceive sensory information on one side of space and contralateral to the lesion18.

In addition to attentional and sensory disruption due to neurodegenerative disorders, the VAN is also associated with neuropsychiatric disorders, like schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)19,20.

In particular, ADHD exhibits abnormal, spontaneous connectivity fluctuations between major networks, such as the DAN, VAN, DMN, and salience network20.

Lower functional connectivity between networks is associated with attentional lapses which characterise the disorder20. Likewise, increased functional connectivity between areas of the VAN and the salience network were found in those suffering ADHD, which Sidlauskaite et al., argued represented an “altered saliency attribution”21. This concept is represented as sufferers weighing non-important stimuli more highly than goal-directed attention, causing spontaneous attentional shifts.

As researchers continue to evaluate the VAN and the implications of changes or damage to the network, studies are also being conducted on other common forms of neuropathology and the implications for patients.

Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often display hyper-connectivity between functional areas critical for attention22. Conversely, adults with ASD demonstrate hypo-connectivity between these areas, suggesting that developmental stages may impact the functional connectivity between brain areas responsible for attentional awareness22,23.

Changes in functional brain circuitry including the VAN may also underpin symptoms present in other disorders. In a comprehensive review and meta-analysis, Sylvestor et al., commented on the heightened stimulus-driven attention which is found in those suffering from anxiety disorders24. Further evidence suggested that a hyperactive VAN is a major contributor to the genesis of these anxiogenic symptoms, including social-phobia and the negative emotional weighting of traditionally neutral information25-26.

Since the efficient, attentional shifting by the VAN is a key element in cognition, the network is highly correlated with many of the brain’s other functional areas. Being a highly active brain network, its wide-ranging correlations make it a key problem area for pathology and resulting disorders. As research continues to study the biomarkers of neurodegenerative and neuropsychological diseases, a greater understanding of the VAN may help lead to more-personalized therapies and treatments.

References
Expand for full list of references
  1. Bernard F, Lemee JM, Mazerand E, Leiber LM, Menei P, ter Minassian A. The ventral attention network: the mirror of the language network in the right brain hemisphere. Journal of Anatomy. 2020;237(4):632-642. doi:10.1111/JOA.13223
  2. Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, Rafal RD. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci. 1984;4(7):1863-1874. doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01863.1984.
  3. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201-215. doi:10.1038/NRN755
  4. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems: Distinct Neural Circuits but Collaborative Roles. The Neuroscientist. 2014;20(2):150. doi:10.1177/1073858413494269
  5. Petersen SE, Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL. PET studies of parietal involvement in spatial attention: comparison of different task types. Can J Exp Psychol. 1994;48(2):319-338. doi:10.1037/1196-1961.48.2.319.
  6. Corbetta M, Shulman GL, Miezin FM, Petersen SE. Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction [published correction appears in Science 1995 Dec 1;270(5241):1423]. Science. 1995;270(5237):802-805. doi:10.1126/science.270.5237.802.
  7. Leonards U, Sunaert S, Van Hecke P, Orban GA. Attention mechanisms in visual search—An fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12(Supplement 2):61-75. doi.org/10.1162/089892900564073.
  8. Weissman DH, Prado J. Heightened activity in a key region of the ventral attention network is linked to reduced activity in a key region of the dorsal attention network during unexpected shifts of covert visual spatial attention. Neuroimage. 2012;61(4):798-804. doi:10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2012.03.032
  9. Treisman AM. Visual perception of features and objects. Defense Technical Information Center website. https://apps.dtic.mil/sti/citations/ADA248578. Published September 14, 1991. Accessed May 9, 2021.
  10. Jonides J. Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind's eye's movement. Atten Perform. https://sites.lsa.umich.edu/jonides-lab/wp-content/uploads/sites/439/2016/10/1981_1.pdf. Published 1981. Accessed March 1, 2021.
  11. Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125-1165. doi:10.1152/jn.00338.2011.
  12. Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20(2):150-159. doi:10.1177/1073858413494269.
  13. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201-215. doi:10.1038/nrn755.
  14. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(26):10046-10051. doi:10.1073/PNAS.0604187103/SUPPL_FILE/04187FIG9.JPG
  15. Allan PG, Briggs RG, Conner AK, O’Neal CM, Bonney PA, Maxwell BD, Baker CM, Burks JD, Sali G, Glenn CA, Sughrue ME. Parcellation-based tractographic modeling of the ventral attention network. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2020;408:116548. doi:10.1016/J.JNS.2019.116548
  16. Li R, Wu X, Fleisher AS, Reiman EM, Chen K, Yao L. Attention-related networks in Alzheimer's disease: a resting functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(5):1076-1088. doi:10.1002/hbm.21269.
  17. Wright WG, Gurfinkel V, King L, Horak F. Parkinson's disease shows perceptuomotor asymmetry unrelated to motor symptoms. Neurosci Lett. 2007;417(1):10-15. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2007.02.016.
  18. Li K, Malhotra PA. Spatial neglect. Pract Neurol. 2015;15:333-339. dx.doi.org/10.1136/practneurol-2015-001115.
  19. Hasenkamp W, James GA, Boshoven W, Duncan E. Altered engagement of attention and default networks during target detection in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2011;125(2-3):169-173. doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.041.
  20. Kucyi A, Hove MJ, Biederman J, Van Dijk KR, Valera EM. Disrupted functional connectivity of cerebellar default network areas in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(9):3373-3386. doi:10.1002/hbm.22850.
  21. Sidlauskaite J, Sonuga-Barke E, Roeyers H, Wiersema JR. Altered intrinsic organisation of brain networks implicated in attentional processes in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a resting-state study of attention, default mode and salience network connectivity. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2016;266(4):349-357. doi:10.1007/S00406-015-0630-0/TABLES/2
  22. Farrant K, Uddin LQ. Atypical developmental of dorsal and ventral attention networks in autism. Dev Sci. 2016;19:550-563. doi.org/10.1111/desc.12359.
  23. Fitzgerald J, Johnson K, Kehoe E, et al. Disrupted functional connectivity in dorsal and ventral attention networks during attention orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2015;8:136-152. doi.org/10.1002/aur.1430.
  24. Sylvester CM, Corbetta M, Raichle ME, Rodebaugh TL, Schlaggar BL, Sheline YI, Zorumski CF, Lenze EJ. Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35(9):527-535. doi:10.1016/J.TINS.2012.04.012
  25. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(34):12569-12574. doi:10.1073/PNAS.0800005105/SUPPL_FILE/0800005105SI.PDF
  26. Onofrj V, Chiarelli AM, Wise R, Colosimo C, Caulo M. Interaction of the salience network, ventral attention network, dorsal attention network and default mode network in neonates and early development of the bottom-up attention system. Brain Structure and Function. 2022;227(5):1843-1856. doi:10.1007/S00429-022-02477-Y/FIGURES/6